Guest guest Posted July 24, 2002 Report Share Posted July 24, 2002 > > > Why We're So Nice: We're Wired to Cooperate > > July 23, 2002 > By NATALIE ANGIER What feels as good as chocolate on the tongue or money in > the bank but won't make you fat or risk a subpoena from the > Securities and Exchange Commission? > > Hard as it may be to believe in these days of infectious > greed and sabers unsheathed, scientists have discovered > that the small, brave act of cooperating with another > person, of choosing trust over cynicism, generosity over > selfishness, makes the brain light up with quiet joy. > > Studying neural activity in young women who were playing a > classic laboratory game called the Prisoner's Dilemma, in > which participants can select from a number of greedy or > cooperative strategies as they pursue financial gain, > researchers found that when the women chose mutualism over > "me-ism," the mental circuitry normally associated with > reward-seeking behavior swelled to life. > > And the longer the women engaged in a cooperative strategy, > the more strongly flowed the blood to the pathways of > pleasure. > > The researchers, performing their work at Emory University > in Atlanta, used magnetic resonance imaging to take what > might be called portraits of the brain on hugs. > > "The results were really surprising to us," said Dr. > Gregory S. Berns, a psychiatrist and an author on the new > report, which appears in the current issue of the journal > Neuron. "We went in expecting the opposite." > > The researchers had thought that the biggest response would > occur in cases where one person cooperated and the other > defected, when the cooperator might feel that she was being > treated unjustly. > > Instead, the brightest signals arose in cooperative > alliances and in those neighborhoods of the brain already > known to respond to desserts, pictures of pretty faces, > money, cocaine and any number of licit or illicit delights. > > > "It's reassuring," Dr. Berns said. "In some ways, it says > that we're wired to cooperate with each other." > > The study is among the first to use M.R.I. technology to > examine social interactions in real time, as opposed to > taking brain images while subjects stared at static > pictures or thought-prescribed thoughts. > > It is also a novel approach to exploring an ancient > conundrum, why are humans so, well, nice? Why are they > willing to cooperate with people whom they barely know and > to do good deeds and to play fair a surprisingly high > percentage of the time? > > Scientists have no trouble explaining the evolution of > competitive behavior. But the depth and breadth of human > altruism, the willingness to forgo immediate personal gain > for the long-term common good, far exceeds behaviors seen > even in other large-brained highly social species like > chimpanzees and dolphins, and it has as such been difficult > to understand. > > "I've pointed out to my students how impressive it is that > you can take a group of young men and women of prime > reproductive age, have them come into a classroom, sit down > and be perfectly comfortable and civil to each other," said > Dr. Peter J. Richerson, a professor of environmental > science and policy at the University of California at Davis > and an influential theorist in the field of cultural > evolution. "If you put 50 male and 50 female chimpanzees > that don't know each other into a lecture hall, it would be > a social explosion." > > Dr. Ernst Fehr of the University of Zurich and colleagues > recently presented findings on the importance of punishment > in maintaining cooperative behavior among humans and the > willingness of people to punish those who commit crimes or > violate norms, even when the chastisers take risks and gain > nothing themselves while serving as ad hoc police. > > In her survey of the management of so-called commons in > small-scale communities where villagers have the right, for > example, to graze livestock on commonly held land, Dr. > Elinor Ostrom of Indiana University found that all > communities have some form of monitoring to gird against > cheating or using more than a fair share of the resource. > > In laboratory games that mimic small-scale commons, Dr. > Richerson said, 20 to 30 percent have to be coerced by a > threat of punishment to cooperate. > > Fear alone is not highly likely to inspire cooperative > behavior to the degree observed among humans. If research > like Dr. Fehr's shows the stick side of the equation, the > newest findings present the neural carrot - people > cooperate because it feels good to do it. > > In the new findings, the researchers studied 36 women from > 20 to 60 years old, many of them students at Emory and > inspired to participate by the promise of monetary rewards. > The scientists chose an all-female sample because so few > brain-imaging studies have looked at only women. Most have > been limited to men or to a mixture of men and women. > > But there is a vast body of non- imaging data that rely on > using the Prisoner's Dilemma. > > "It's a simple and elegant model for reciprocity," said Dr. > James K. Rilling, an author on the Neuron paper who is at > Princeton. "It's been referred to as the E. coli of social > psychology." > > >From past results, the researchers said, one can assume > that neuro- imaging studies of men playing the game would > be similar to their new findings with women. > > The basic structure of the trial had two women meet each > other briefly ahead of time. One was placed in the scanner > while the other remained outside the scanning room. The two > interacted by computer, playing about 20 rounds of the > game. In every round, each player pressed a button to > indicate whether she would "cooperate" or "defect." Her > answer would be shown on-screen to the other player. > > The monetary awards were apportioned after each round. If > one player defected and the other cooperated, the defector > earned $3 and the cooperator nothing. If both chose to > cooperate, each earned $2. If both opted to defect, each > earned $1. > > Hence, mutual cooperation from start to finish was a far > more profitable strategy, at $40 a woman, than complete > mutual defection, which gave each $20. > > The risk that a woman took each time she became greedy for > a little bit more was that the cooperative strategy would > fall apart and that both would emerge the poorer. > > In some cases, both women were allowed to pursue any > strategy that they chose. In other cases, the non- scanned > woman would be a "confederate" with the researchers, > instructed, unbeknown to the scanned subject, to defect > after three consecutive rounds of cooperation, the better > to keep things less rarefied and pretty and more lifelike > and gritty. > > In still other experiments, the woman in the scanner played > a computer and knew that her partner was a machine. In > other tests, women played a computer but thought that it > was a human. > > The researchers found that as a rule the freely > strategizing women cooperated. Even occasional episodes of > defection, whether from free strategizers or confederates, > were not necessarily fatal to an alliance. > > "The social bond could be reattained easily if the defector > chose to cooperate in the next couple of rounds," another > author of the report, Dr. Clinton D. Kilts, said, "although > the one who had originally been `betrayed' might be wary > from then on." > > As a result of the episodic defections, the average > per-experiment take for the participants was in the $30's. > "Some pairs, though, got locked into mutual defection," Dr. > Rilling said. > > Analyzing the scans, the researchers found that in rounds > of cooperation, two broad areas of the brain were > activated, both rich in neurons able to respond to > dopamine, the brain chemical famed for its role in > addictive behaviors. > > One is the anteroventral striatum in the middle of the > brain right above the spinal cord. Experiments with rats > have shown that when electrodes are placed in the striatum, > the animals will repeatedly press a bar to stimulate the > electrodes, apparently receiving such pleasurable feedback > that they will starve to death rather than stop pressing > the bar. > > Another region activated during cooperation was the > orbitofrontal cortex in the region right above the eyes. In > addition to being part of the reward-processing system, Dr. > Rilling said, it is also involved in impulse control. > > "Every round, you're confronted with the possibility of > getting an extra dollar by defecting," he said. "The choice > to cooperate requires impulse control." > > Significantly, the reward circuitry of the women was > considerably less responsive when they knew that they were > playing against a computer. The thought of a human bond, > but not mere monetary gain, was the source of contentment > on display. > > In concert with the imaging results, the women, when asked > afterward for summaries of how they felt during the games, > often described feeling good when they cooperated and > expressed positive feelings of camaraderie toward their > playing partners. > > Assuming that the urge to cooperate is to some extent > innate among humans and reinforced by the brain's feel-good > circuitry, the question of why it arose remains unclear. > Anthropologists have speculated that it took teamwork for > humanity's ancestors to hunt large game or gather difficult > plant foods or rear difficult children. So the capacity to > cooperate conferred a survival advantage on our forebears. > > Yet as with any other trait, the willingness to abide by > the golden rule and to be a good citizen and not cheat and > steal from one's neighbors is not uniformly distributed. > > "If we put some C.E.O.'s in here, I'd like to see how they > respond," Dr. Kilts said. "Maybe they wouldn't find a > positive social interaction rewarding at all." > > A Prisoner's Dilemma indeed. > > > http://www.nytimes.com/2002/07/23/health/psychology/23COOP.html?ex=102848689 8&ei=1&en=ad636fcd13a54508 > > > > HOW TO ADVERTISE > > For information on advertising in e-mail newsletters > or other creative advertising opportunities with The > New York Times on the Web, please contact > onlinesales (AT) nytimes (DOT) com or visit our online media > kit at http://www.nytimes.com/adinfo > > For general information about NYTimes.com, write to > help (AT) nytimes (DOT) com. > > Copyright 2002 The New York Times Company > Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.