Jump to content
IndiaDivine.org

Health: Dr. Drug Rep

Rate this topic


Guest guest

Recommended Posts

November 25, 2007

 

Dr. Drug Rep

 

By DANIEL CARLAT

 

I. Faculty Development

 

On a blustery fall New England day in 2001, a friendly representative

from Wyeth Pharmaceuticals came into my office in Newburyport, Mass.,

and made me an offer I found hard to refuse. He asked me if I'd like

to give talks to other doctors about using Effexor XR for treating

depression. He told me that I would go around to doctors' offices

during lunchtime and talk about some of the features of Effexor. It

would be pretty easy. Wyeth would provide a set of slides and even pay

for me to attend a speaker's training session, and he quickly floated

some numbers. I would be paid $500 for one-hour " Lunch and Learn "

talks at local doctors' offices, or $750 if I had to drive an hour. I

would be flown to New York for a " faculty-development program, " where

I would be pampered in a Midtown hotel for two nights and would be

paid an additional " honorarium. "

 

I thought about his proposition. I had a busy private practice in

psychiatry, specializing in psychopharmacology. I was quite familiar

with Effexor, since I had read recent studies showing that it might be

slightly more effective than S.S.R.I.'s, the most commonly prescribed

antidepressants: the Prozacs, Paxils and Zolofts of the world.

S.S.R.I. stands for selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, referring

to the fact that these drugs increase levels of the neurotransmitter

serotonin, a chemical in the brain involved in regulating moods.

Effexor, on the other hand, was being marketed as a dual reuptake

inhibitor, meaning that it increases both serotonin and

norepinephrine, another neurotransmitter. The theory promoted by Wyeth

was that two neurotransmitters are better than one, and that Effexor

was more powerful and effective than S.S.R.I.'s.

 

I had already prescribed Effexor to several patients, and it seemed to

work as well as the S.S.R.I.'s. If I gave talks to primary-care

doctors about Effexor, I reasoned, I would be doing nothing unethical.

It was a perfectly effective treatment option, with some data to

suggest advantages over its competitors. The Wyeth rep was simply

suggesting that I discuss some of the data with other doctors. Sure,

Wyeth would benefit, but so would other doctors, who would become more

educated about a good medication.

 

A few weeks later, my wife and I walked through the luxurious lobby of

the Millennium Hotel in Midtown Manhattan. At the reception desk, when

I gave my name, the attendant keyed it into the computer and said,

with a dazzling smile: " Hello, Dr. Carlat, I see that you are with the

Wyeth conference. Here are your materials. "

 

She handed me a folder containing the schedule of talks, an invitation

to various dinners and receptions and two tickets to a Broadway

musical. " Enjoy your stay, doctor. " I had no doubt that I would,

though I felt a gnawing at the edge of my conscience. This seemed like

a lot of money to lavish on me just so that I could provide some

education to primary-care doctors in a small town north of Boston.

 

The next morning, the conference began. There were a hundred or so

other psychiatrists from different parts of the U.S. I recognized a

couple of the attendees, including an acquaintance I hadn't seen in a

while. I'd heard that he moved to another state and was making a

bundle of money, but nobody seemed to know exactly how.

 

I joined him at his table and asked him what he had been up to. He

said he had a busy private practice and had given a lot of talks for

Warner-Lambert, a company that had since been acquired by Pfizer. His

talks were on Neurontin, a drug that was approved for epilepsy but

that my friend had found helpful for bipolar disorder in his practice.

(In 2004, Warner-Lambert pleaded guilty to illegally marketing

Neurontin for unapproved uses. It is illegal for companies to pay

doctors to promote so-called off-label uses.)

 

I knew about Neurontin and had prescribed it occasionally for bipolar

disorder in my practice, though I had never found it very helpful. A

recent study found that it worked no better than a placebo for this

condition. I asked him if he really thought Neurontin worked for

bipolar, and he said that he felt it was " great for some patients " and

that he used it " all the time. " Given my clinical experiences with the

drug, I wondered whether his positive opinion had been influenced by

the money he was paid to give talks.

 

But I put those questions aside as we gulped down our coffees and took

seats in a large lecture room. On the agenda were talks from some of

the most esteemed academics in the field, authors of hundreds of

articles in the major psychiatric journals. They included Michael

Thase, of the University of Pittsburgh and the researcher who

single-handedly put Effexor on the map with a meta-analysis, and

Norman Sussman, a professor of psychiatry at New York University, who

was master of ceremonies.

 

Thase strode to the lectern first in order to describe his

groundbreaking work synthesizing data from more than 2,000 patients

who had been enrolled in studies comparing Effexor with S.S.R.I.'s. At

this time, with his Effexor study a topic of conversation in the

mental-health world, Thase was one of the most well known and well

respected psychiatrists in the United States. He cut a captivating

figure onstage: tall and slim, dynamic, incredibly articulate and a

master of the research craft.

 

He began by reviewing the results of the meta-analysis that had the

psychiatric world abuzz. After carefully pooling and processing data

from eight separate clinical trials, Thase published a truly

significant finding: Effexor caused a 45 percent remission rate in

patients in contrast to the S.S.R.I. rate of 35 percent and the

placebo rate of 25 percent. It was the first time one antidepressant

was shown to be more effective than any other. Previously,

psychiatrists chose antidepressants based on a combination of

guesswork, gut feeling and tailoring a drug's side effects to a

patient's symptom profile. If Effexor was truly more effective than

S.S.R.I.'s, it would amount to a revolution in psychiatric practice

and a potential windfall for Wyeth.

 

One impressive aspect of Thase's presentation was that he was not

content to rest on his laurels; rather he raised a series of potential

criticisms of his results and then rebutted them convincingly. For

example, skeptics had pointed out that Thase was a paid consultant to

Wyeth and that both of his co-authors were employees of the company.

Thase responded that he had requested and had received all of the

company's data and had not cherry-picked from those studies most

favorable for Effexor. This was a significant point, because companies

sometimes withhold negative data from publication in medical journals.

For example, in 2004, GlaxoSmithKline was sued by Eliot Spitzer, who

was then the New York attorney general, for suppressing data hinting

that Paxil causes suicidal thoughts in children. The company settled

the case and agreed to make clinical-trial results public.

 

Another objection was that while the study was billed as comparing

Effexor with S.S.R.I.'s in general, in fact most of the data compared

Effexor with one specific S.S.R.I.: Prozac. Perhaps Effexor was,

indeed, more effective than Prozac; this did not necessarily mean that

it was more effective than the other S.S.R.I.'s in common use. But

Thase announced that since the original study, he had analyzed data on

Paxil and other meds and also found differences in remission rates.

 

For his study, Thase chose what was at that time an unusual measure of

antidepressant improvement: " remission, " rather than the more standard

measure, " response. " In clinical antidepressant trials, a " response "

is defined as a 50 percent improvement in depressive symptoms, as

measured by the Hamilton depression scale. Thus, if a patient enters a

study scoring a 24 on the Hamilton (which would be a moderate degree

of depression), he or she would have " responded " if the final score,

after treatment, was 12 or less.

 

Remission, on the other hand, is defined as " complete " recovery. While

you might think that a patient would have to score a 0 on the Hamilton

to be in remission, in fact very few people score that low, no matter

how deliriously happy they are. Instead, researchers come up with

various cutoff scores for remission. Thase chose a cutoff score of 7

or below.

 

In his study, he emphasized the remission rates and not the response

rates. As I listened to his presentation, I wondered why. Was it

because he felt that remission was the only really meaningful outcome

by which to compare drugs? Or was it because using remission made

Effexor look more impressive than response did? Thase indirectly

addressed this issue in his paper by pointing out that even when

remission was defined in different ways, with different cutoff points,

Effexor beat the S.S.R.I.'s every time. That struck me as a pretty

convincing endorsement of Wyeth's antidepressant.

 

The next speaker, Norm Sussman, took the baton from Thase and explored

the concept of remission in more detail. Sussman's job was to

systematically go through the officially sanctioned " slide deck " —

slides provided to us by Wyeth, which we were expected to use during

our own presentations.

 

If Thase was the riveting academic, Sussman was the engaging populist,

translating some of the drier research concepts into terms that our

primary-care-physician audiences would understand. Sussman exhorted us

not to be satisfied with response and encouraged us to set the bar

higher. " Is the patient doing everything they were doing before they

got depressed? " he asked. " Are they doing it even better? That's

remission. " To further persuade us, he highlighted a slide showing

that patients who made it all the way to remission are less likely to

relapse to another depressive episode than patients who merely

responded. And for all its methodological limitations, it was a slide

that I would become well acquainted with, as I would use it over and

over again in my own talks.

 

When it came to side effects, Effexor's greatest liability was that it

could cause hypertension, a side effect not shared by S.S.R.I.'s.

Sussman showed us some data from the clinical trials, indicating that

at lower doses, about 3 percent of patients taking Effexor had

hypertension as compared with about 2 percent of patients assigned to

a placebo. There was only a 1 percent difference between Effexor and

placebo, he commented, and pointed out that treating high blood

pressure might be a small price to pay for relief from depression.

 

It was an accurate reading of the data, and I remember finding it a

convincing defense of Effexor's safety. As I look back at my notes

now, however, I notice that another way of describing the same numbers

would have been to say that Effexor leads to a 50 percent greater rate

of hypertension than a placebo. Framed this way, Effexor looks more

hazardous.

 

And so it went for the rest of the afternoon.

 

Was I swallowing the message whole? Certainly not. I knew that this

was hardly impartial medical education, and that we were being fed a

marketing line. But when you are treated like the anointed, wined and

dined in Manhattan and placed among the leaders of the field, you

inevitably put some of your critical faculties on hold. I was truly

impressed with Effexor's remission numbers, and like any physician, I

was hopeful that something new and different had been introduced to my

quiver of therapeutic options.

 

At the end of the last lecture, we were all handed envelopes as we

left the conference room. Inside were checks for $750. It was time to

enjoy ourselves in the city.

 

II. The Art and Science of Detailing

 

Pharmaceutical " detailing " is the term used to describe those sales

visits in which drug reps go to doctors' offices to describe the

benefits of a specific drug. Once I returned to my Newburyport office

from New York, a couple of voice-mail messages from local Wyeth reps

were already waiting for me, inviting me to give some presentations at

local doctors' offices. I was about to begin my speaking — and

detailing — career in earnest.

 

How many doctors speak for drug companies? We don't know for sure, but

one recent study indicates that at least 25 percent of all doctors in

the United States receive drug money for lecturing to physicians or

for helping to market drugs in other ways. This meant that I was about

to join some 200,000 American physicians who are being paid by

companies to promote their drugs. I felt quite flattered to have been

recruited, and I assumed that the rep had picked me because of some

special personal or professional quality.

 

The first talk I gave brought me back to earth rather quickly. I

distinctly remember the awkwardness of walking into my first waiting

room. The receptionist slid the glass partition open and asked if I

had an appointment.

 

" Actually, I'm here to meet with the doctor. "

 

" Oh, O.K. And is that a scheduled appointment? "

 

" I'm here to give a talk. "

 

A light went on. " Oh, are you part of the drug lunch? "

 

Regardless of how I preferred to think of myself (an educator, a

psychiatrist, a consultant), I was now classified as one facet of a

lunch helping to pitch a drug, a convincing sidekick to help the sales

rep. Eventually, with an internal wince, I began to introduce myself

as " Dr. Carlat, here for the Wyeth lunch. "

 

The drug rep who arranged the lunch was always there, usually an

attractive, vivacious woman with platters of gourmet sandwiches in

tow. Hungry doctors and their staff of nurses and receptionists would

filter into the lunch room, grateful for free food.

 

Once there was a critical mass (and crucially, once the M.D.'s

arrived), I was given the go-ahead by the Wyeth reps to start. I dove

into my talk, going through a handout that I created, based on the

official slide deck. I discussed the importance of remission, the

basics of the Thase study showing the advantage of Effexor, how to

dose the drug, the side effects, and I added a quick review of the

other common antidepressants.

 

While I still had some doubts, I continued to be impressed by the 10

percent advantage in remission rates that Effexor held over

S.S.R.I.'s; that advantage seemed significant enough to overcome

Effexor's more prominent side effects. Yes, I was highlighting

Effexor's selling points and playing down its disadvantages, and I

knew it. But was my salesmanship going to bring harm to anybody? It

seemed unlikely. The worst case was that Effexor was no more effective

than anything else; it certainly was no less effective.

 

During my first few talks, I worried a lot about my performance. Was I

too boring? Did the doctors see me as sleazy? Did the Wyeth reps find

me sufficiently persuasive? But the day after my talks, I would get a

call or an e-mail message from the rep saying that I did a great job,

that the doctor was impressed and that they wanted to use me more.

Indeed, I started receiving more and more invitations from other reps,

and I soon had talks scheduled every week. I learned later that Wyeth

and other companies have speaker-evaluation systems. After my talks,

the reps would fill out a questionnaire rating my performance, which

quickly became available to other Wyeth reps throughout the area.

 

As the reps became comfortable with me, they began to see me more as a

sales colleague. I received faxes before talks preparing me for

particular doctors. One note informed me that the physician we'd be

visiting that day was a " decile 6 doctor and is not prescribing any

Effexor XR, so please tailor accordingly. There is also one more doc

in the practice that we are not familiar with. " The term " decile 6 " is

drug-rep jargon for a doctor who prescribes a lot of medications. The

higher the " decile " (in a range from 1 to 10), the higher the

prescription volume, and the more potentially lucrative that doctor

could be for the company.

 

A note from another rep reminded me of a scene from " Mission:

Impossible. " " Dr. Carlat: Our main target, Dr. , is an internist. He

spreads his usage among three antidepressants, Celexa, Zoloft and

Paxil, at about 25-30 percent each. He is currently using about 6

percent Effexor XR. Our access is very challenging with lunches six

months out. " This doctor's schedule of lunches was filled with reps

from other companies; it would be vital to make our sales visit

count.+

 

Naïve as I was, I found myself astonished at the level of detail that

drug companies were able to acquire about doctors' prescribing habits.

I asked my reps about it; they told me that they received printouts

tracking local doctors' prescriptions every week. The process is

called " prescription data-mining, " in which specialized

pharmacy-information companies (like IMS Health and Verispan) buy

prescription data from local pharmacies, repackage it, then sell it to

pharmaceutical companies. This information is then passed on to the

drug reps, who use it to tailor their drug-detailing strategies. This

may include deciding which physicians to aim for, as my Wyeth reps

did, but it can help sales in other ways. For example, Shahram Ahari,

a former drug rep for Eli Lilly (the maker of Prozac) who is now a

researcher at the University of California at San Francisco's School

of Pharmacy, said in an article in The Washington Post that as a drug

rep he would use this data to find out which doctors were prescribing

Prozac's competitors, like Effexor. Then he would play up specific

features of Prozac that contrasted favorably with the other drug, like

the ease with which patients can get off Prozac, as compared with the

hard time they can have withdrawing from Effexor.

 

The American Medical Association is also a key player in prescription

data-mining. Pharmacies typically will not release doctors' names to

the data-mining companies, but they will release their Drug

Enforcement Agency numbers. The A.M.A. licenses its file of U.S.

physicians, allowing the data-mining companies to match up D.E.A.

numbers to specific physicians. The A.M.A. makes millions in

information-leasing money.

 

Once drug companies have identified the doctors, they must woo them.

In the April 2007 issue of the journal PLoS Medicine, Dr. Adriane

Fugh-Berman of Georgetown teamed up with Ahari (the former drug rep)

to describe the myriad techniques drug reps use to establish

relationships with physicians, including inviting them to a speaker's

meeting. These can serve to cement a positive a relationship between

the rep and the doctor. This relationship is crucial, they say, since

" drug reps increase drug sales by influencing physicians, and they do

so with finely titrated doses of friendship. "

 

III. Uncomfortable Moments

 

I gave many talks over the ensuing several months, and I gradually

became more comfortable with the process. Each setting was somewhat

different. Sometimes I spoke to a crowded conference room with several

physicians, nurses and other clinical staff. Other times, I sat at a

small lunch table with only one other physician (plus the rep), having

what amounted to a conversation about treating depression. My basic

Effexor spiel was similar in the various settings, with the focus on

remission and the Thase data.

 

Meanwhile, I was keeping up with new developments in the research

literature related to Effexor, and not all of the news was positive.

For example, as more data came out comparing Effexor with S.S.R.I.'s

other than Prozac, the Effexor remission advantage became slimmer —

more like 5 percent instead of the originally reported 10 percent.

Statistically, this 5 percent advantage meant that only one out of 20

patients would potentially do better on Effexor than S.S.R.I.'s — much

less compelling than the earlier proportion of one out of 10.

 

I also became aware of other critiques of the original Thase

meta-analysis. For example, some patients enrolled in the original

Effexor studies took S.S.R.I.'s in the past and presumably had not

responded well. This meant that the study population may have been

enriched with patients who were treatment-resistant to S.S.R.I.'s,

giving Effexor an inherent advantage.

 

I didn't mention any of this in my talks, partly because none of it

had been included in official company slides, and partly because I was

concerned that the reps wouldn't invite me to give talks if I divulged

any negative information. But I was beginning to struggle with the

ethics of my silence.

 

One of my most uncomfortable moments came when I gave a presentation

to a large group of psychiatrists. I was in the midst of wrapping up

my talk with some information about Effexor and blood pressure.

Referring to a large study paid for by Wyeth, I reported that patients

are liable to develop hypertension only if they are taking Effexor at

doses higher than 300 milligrams per day.

 

" Really? " one psychiatrist in the room said. " I've seen hypertension

at lower doses in my patients. "

 

" I suppose it can happen, but it's rare at doses that are commonly

used for depression. "

 

He looked at me, frowned and shook his head. " That hasn't been my experience. "

 

I reached into my folder where I kept some of the key Effexor studies

in case such questions arose.

 

According to this study of 3,744 patients, the rate of high blood

pressure was 2.2 percent in the placebo group, and 2.9 percent in the

group of patients who had taken daily doses of Effexor no larger than

300 milligrams. Patients taking more than 300 milligrams had a 9

percent risk of hypertension. As I went through the numbers with the

doctor, however, I felt unsettled. I started talking faster, a sure

sign of nervousness for me.

 

Driving home, I went back over the talk in my mind. I knew I had not

lied — I had reported the data exactly as they were reported in the

paper. But still, I had spun the results of the study in the most

positive way possible, and I had not talked about the limitations of

the data. I had not, for example, mentioned that if you focused

specifically on patients taking between 200 and 300 milligrams per

day, a commonly prescribed dosage range, you found a 3.7 percent

incidence of hypertension. While this was not a statistically

significant higher rate than the placebo, it still hinted that such

moderate doses could, indeed, cause hypertension. Nor had I mentioned

the fact that since the data were derived from placebo-controlled

clinical trials, the patients were probably not representative of the

patients seen in most real practices. Patients who are very old or who

have significant medical problems are excluded from such studies. But

real-world patients may well be at higher risk to develop hypertension

on Effexor. +

 

I realized that in my canned talks, I was blithely minimizing the

hypertension risks, conveniently overlooking the fact that

hypertension is a dangerous condition and not one to be trifled with.

Why, I began to wonder, would anyone prescribe an antidepressant that

could cause hypertension when there were many other alternatives? And

why wasn't I asking this obvious question out loud during my talks?

 

I felt rattled. That psychiatrist's frown stayed with me — a mixture

of skepticism and contempt. I wondered if he saw me for what I feared

I had become — a drug rep with an M.D. I began to think that the money

was affecting my critical judgement. I was willing to dance around the

truth in order to make the drug reps happy. Receiving $750 checks for

chatting with some doctors during a lunch break was such easy money

that it left me giddy. Like an addiction, it was very hard to give up.

 

There was another problem: one of Effexor's side effects. Patients who

stopped the medication were calling their doctors and reporting

symptoms like severe dizziness and lightheadedness, bizarre

electric-shock sensations in their heads, insomnia, sadness and

tearfulness. Some patients thought they were having strokes or nervous

breakdowns and were showing up in emergency rooms. Gradually, however,

it became clear that these were " withdrawal " symptoms. These were

particularly common problems with Effexor because it has a short

half-life, a measure of the time it takes the body to metabolize half

of the total amount of a drug in the bloodstream. Paxil, another short

half-life antidepressant, caused similar problems.

 

At the Wyeth meeting in New York, these withdrawal effects were

mentioned in passing, though we were assured that Effexor withdrawal

symptoms were uncommon and could usually be avoided by tapering down

the dose very slowly. But in my practice, that strategy often did not

work, and patients were having a very hard time coming off Effexor in

order to start a trial of a different antidepressant.

 

I wrestled with how to handle this issue in my Effexor talks, since I

believed it was a significant disadvantage of the drug. Psychiatrists

frequently have to switch medications because of side effects or lack

of effectiveness, and anticipating this potential need to change

medications plays into our initial choice of a drug. Knowing that

Effexor was hard to give up made me think twice about prescribing it

in the first place.

 

During my talks, I found myself playing both sides of the issue,

making sure to mention that withdrawal symptoms could be severe but

assuring doctors that they could " usually " be avoided. Was I lying?

Not really, since there were no solid published data, and indeed some

patients had little problem coming off Effexor. But was I tweaking and

pruning the truth in order to stay positive about the product?

Definitely. And how did I rationalize this? I convinced myself that I

had told " most " of the truth and that the potential negative

consequences of this small truth " gap " were too trivial to worry

about.

 

As the months went on, I developed more and more reservations about

recommending that Effexor be used as a " first line " drug before trying

the S.S.R.I.'s. Not only were the newer comparative data less

impressive, but the studies were short-term, lasting only 6 to 12

weeks. It seemed entirely possible that if the clinical trials had

been longer — say, six months — S.S.R.I.'s would have caught up with

Effexor. Effexor was turning out to be an antidepressant that might

have a very slight effectiveness advantage over S.S.R.I.'s but that

caused high blood pressure and had prolonged withdrawal symptoms.

 

At my next Lunch and Learn, I mentioned toward the end of my

presentation that data in support of Effexor were mainly short-term,

and that there was a possibility that S.S.R.I.'s were just as

effective. I felt reckless, but I left the office with a restored

sense of integrity.

 

Several days later, I was visited by the same district manager who

first offered me the speaking job. Pleasant as always, he said: " My

reps told me that you weren't as enthusiastic about our product at

your last talk. I told them that even Dr. Carlat can't hit a home run

every time. Have you been sick? "

 

At that moment, I decided my career as an industry-sponsored speaker

was over. The manager's message couldn't be clearer: I was being paid

to enthusiastically endorse their drug. Once I stopped doing that, I

was of little value to them, no matter how much " medical education " I

provided.

 

IV. Life After Drug Money

 

A year after starting my educational talks for drug companies (I had

also given two talks for Forest Pharmaceuticals, pushing the

antidepressant Lexapro), I quit. I had made about $30,000 in

supplemental income from these talks, a significant addition to the

$140,000 or so I made from my private practice. Now I publish a

medical-education newsletter for psychiatrists that is not financed by

the pharmaceutical industry and that tries to critically assess drug

research and marketing claims. I still see patients, and I still

prescribe Effexor. I don't prescribe it as frequently as I used to,

but I have seen many patients turn their lives around because they

responded to this drug and to nothing else. +

 

In 2002, the drug industry's trade group adopted voluntary guidelines

limiting some of the more lavish benefits to doctors. While the

guidelines still allow all-expenses-paid trips for physicians to

attend meetings at fancy hotels, they no longer pay for spouses to

attend the dinners or hand out tickets to musicals. In an e-mail

message, a Wyeth spokesman wrote that Wyeth employees must follow that

code and " our own Wyeth policies, which, in some cases, exceed " the

trade group's code.

 

Looking back on the year I spent speaking for Wyeth, I've asked myself

if my work as a company speaker led me to do bad things. Did I

contribute to faulty medical decision making? Did my advice lead

doctors to make inappropriate drug choices, and did their patients

suffer needlessly?

 

Maybe. I'm sure I persuaded many physicians to prescribe Effexor,

potentially contributing to blood-pressure problems and withdrawal

symptoms. On the other hand, it's possible that some of those patients

might have gained more relief from their depression and anxiety than

they would have if they had been started on an S.S.R.I. Not likely,

but possible.

 

I still allow drug reps to visit my office and give me their pitches.

While these visits are short on useful medical information, they do

allow me to keep up with trends in drug marketing. Recently, a rep

from Bristol-Myers Squibb came into my office and invited me to a

dinner program on the antipsychotic Abilify.

 

" I think it will be a great program, Dr. Carlat, " he said. " Would you

like to come? " I glanced at the invitation. I recognized the name of

the speaker, a prominent and widely published psychiatrist flown in

from another state. The restaurant was one of the finest in town.

 

I was tempted. The wine, the great food, the proximity to a famous

researcher — why not rejoin that inner circle of the select for an

evening? But then I flashed to a memory of myself five years earlier,

standing at a lectern and clearing my throat at the beginning of a

drug-company presentation. I vividly remembered my sensations — the

careful monitoring of what I would say, the calculations of how frank

I should be.

 

" No, " I said, as I handed the rep back the invitation. " I don't think

I can make it. But thanks anyway. "

 

Daniel Carlat is an assistant clinical professor of psychiatry at

Tufts University School of Medicine and the publisher of The Carlat

Psychiatry Report.

 

Copyright 2007 The New York Times Company

Link to comment
Share on other sites

Just change the names and the faces and you have my exact observational

experience when I worked in marketing for another pharma.

 

Dale

The people who make a difference are not the ones with the credentials, but the

ones with the concern. ~ Max Lucado

 

Believe in the power of words at birth: http://tinyurl.com/3748nc

Link to comment
Share on other sites

Join the conversation

You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.

Guest
Reply to this topic...

×   Pasted as rich text.   Paste as plain text instead

  Only 75 emoji are allowed.

×   Your link has been automatically embedded.   Display as a link instead

×   Your previous content has been restored.   Clear editor

×   You cannot paste images directly. Upload or insert images from URL.

Loading...
×
×
  • Create New...