Guest guest Posted September 17, 2008 Report Share Posted September 17, 2008 Kim, thanks for that. Whilst an 11-year old and experiencing fishing just like Bidda did, I can relate. My own un-scientific conclusion at the time disagreed with the Roslin Institute¹s; fish appeared to feel pain when being bashed with a hammer, or when gutted when alive. Duh. Perhaps unrelated except that this article deals with Australian fish, is this bizarro report from the NYT: (http://www.nytimes.com/2006/10/01/arts/design/01voge.html?pagewanted=1) ³Swimming With Famous Dead Sharks² about Damien Hirst¹s strange obsession with dead animals as artwork. WTF? Jigs in Nepal On 9/17/08 4:38 AM, " Kim Bartlett " <anpeople wrote: > > > > http://www.smh.com.au/news/opinion/the-catch-is-this-sport-could-well-be-cruel > /2008/09/15/1221330741163.html > > The catch is this sport could well be cruel > > Bidda Jones > September 16, 2008 > > When I was 11, I caught my first fish. It turned out to be my last > one, too. After a short wait, a small, flapping, brownish, shiny fish > was dangling at the end of my handline, and became my responsibility. > It was my job to do the killing. I can't remember what I hit the fish > with, but after 10 or so blows to what I hoped was its head (I had my > eyes shut), it was still flapping around in the boat. I remember its > mouth gaping and shutting as it struggled to stay alive. My father > finally killed it, and cooked it for dinner. > These days I spend my working life telling other people how they > should treat animals. And that, at least for me, includes fish. > Despite knowing a lot about fish, we still argue about their capacity > to feel pain. While we have an increased capacity to empathise with > other animals that sound and behave like us, the big problem with > fish is that they're just too different. > And because of that we don't really worry about what we do to them. > In parts of Australia, until very recently, fish didn't count when it > came to animal welfare legislation - the legal definition of an > animal excluded them. > (There are still some weird anomalies, such as in the Northern > Territory where you can be prosecuted for being cruel to a fish > living in captivity, but not if it's in the wild.) > Angling is one of the most popular recreational pastimes in > Australia, the United States, Britain and a lot of other places. It's > everywhere; it is totally establishment. At least 5 million > Australians are involved, which is why the debate over whether fish > feel pain makes people nervous. Really nervous. > Proving fish don't feel pain, at least in the way humans feel pain, > seems to have become a personal mission for one American academic, > James Rose, a professor in the Department of Zoology and Physiology > at the University of Wyoming, Laramie. Rose is doing his scientific > best to make sure no fuzzy-minded, tree-hugging animal lover stands > between the men in waders and their potential catch. > Rose argues that because fish don't have an adequately developed > forebrain neocortex (the bit that does all the clever stuff), they > are incapable of consciousness, and therefore cannot feel pain. > But while the neocortex is important in shaping the degree of > consciousness of an animal, we don't really know where in the animal > kingdom we can draw the line between species that do or don't > experience consciousness. Nor do we yet fully understand what > consciousness is. > For example, recent studies of patients diagnosed as being in a > persistent vegetative states have found that in some cases their > brains respond in a similar way to those of a conscious person. > Conversely, Rose uses the case of Terri Schiavo, who was diagnosed in > 1990 as being in a persistent vegetative state. Schiavo's parents > claimed her movements and reaction to noxious stimuli were evidence > she was still conscious. > The neurologists said she was unconscious, her reactions were > reflexive and her condition irreversible. Following her death in > 2005, the neurologists' diagnosis was confirmed by autopsy. Rose's > point is that we should not be misled by what we observe: what > animals do means nothing in itself, since their actions may be in > response to a reflex over which they have no conscious control. The > converse, though, is that we should not be misled by what we can't > observe. > Rose accepts fish have nocioceptors, receptors which respond to > painful stimuli and which are similar in structure to our own. But, > says Rose, having nocioceptors isn't enough to join the pain club, > because the reflex is common to most animals and does not require a > complex nervous system. To be a fully fledged member you must > experience or feel pain, and to do that you need consciousness. > In contrast, Lynne Sneddon, from the Roslin Institute in Scotland, > and her colleagues, found that after rainbow trout had acetic acid or > bee venom injected into their lips, they increased their breathing > rate, were slower to feed, rocked from side to side and rubbed their > lips on the tank floor and walls. > Where does that leave the big fish debate? The no-pain believers say > until we prove that fish have consciousness, we cannot say they can > feel pain. > The opposing camp says because fish demonstrate behaviour indicative > of experiencing pain, we should assume they do. We don't know what > fish feel, but if they lose their appetite and act like they've got a > sore mouth after being jabbed in the lips with bee venom, that's good > enough for me. It hurts - somehow. > > Bidda Jones is chief scientist of RSPCA Australia. This is an edited > extract from her chapter in The Finlay Lloyd Book About ANIMALS, to > be published on October 4. -- Paul Reitman, CEO Phoenix Studios Nepal Mobile: 9841589797 www.phoenixstudios.com.np/corporate Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.